MESSAGE
DATE | 2009-03-03 |
FROM | Ronny Abraham
|
SUBJECT | Re: [NYLXS - HANGOUT] Free Software Cooperative - it is in the genes
|
From lest-hangout-at-mrbrklyn.com Tue Mar 3 14:10:36 2009 Received: from www2.mrbrklyn.com (localhost [127.0.0.1]) by www2.mrbrklyn.com (8.13.1/8.13.1/SuSE Linux 0.7) with ESMTP id n23JAYUs022041 for ; Tue, 3 Mar 2009 14:10:36 -0500 Received: (from majordomo-at-localhost) by www2.mrbrklyn.com (8.13.1/8.13.1/Submit) id n23JAYu2022040 for hangout-outgoings; Tue, 3 Mar 2009 14:10:34 -0500 X-Authentication-Warning: www2.mrbrklyn.com: majordomo set sender to lest-hangout-at-nylxs.com using -f Received: from mail-gx0-f170.google.com (mail-gx0-f170.google.com [209.85.217.170]) by www2.mrbrklyn.com (8.13.1/8.13.1/SuSE Linux 0.7) with ESMTP id n23JAVou022037 for ; Tue, 3 Mar 2009 14:10:33 -0500 Received: by gxk18 with SMTP id 18so5859276gxk.5 for ; Tue, 03 Mar 2009 11:10:30 -0800 (PST) DKIM-Signature: v=1; a=rsa-sha256; c=relaxed/relaxed; d=gmail.com; s=gamma; h=domainkey-signature:mime-version:received:in-reply-to:references :date:message-id:subject:from:to:content-type; bh=zpwCCKobVR1PamheAwskIZxkonj87LzpU0Zta94MpSA=; b=WdqR0hUfbxCd9aanxecPgqGda6SVT2ewmRXC5BfHSyjqvRMRC1Ysq/NXmqHOJJG0Et xWwi35B+P1M2x3phEkidPTNSww0vvZIJbbTJWD66KtFHKRwE6PVN4WS8LkaJnzCrIWi7 OwV+jLp3MHYOW8wpJLOFgYnqO6I6DLICyChcI= DomainKey-Signature: a=rsa-sha1; c=nofws; d=gmail.com; s=gamma; h=mime-version:in-reply-to:references:date:message-id:subject:from:to :content-type; b=O7TbVFE1OGKdfRPKmVR2RYW/MD/QX4g/qyYzrkC6p69HdxpXhr1+AVlpuwIzV4x8AI Pavi16W0XVBqLMzTrwQFWnU9naXWxl/1SVMYtYJQxlJCxoxKm8iNxm9K79w4hzLIbYb7 g0g4eOa4Zoe9Ad8x5tIkjFRwFNvMAB3/XNt7E= MIME-Version: 1.0 Received: by 10.100.57.13 with SMTP id f13mr5915532ana.143.1236107429998; Tue, 03 Mar 2009 11:10:29 -0800 (PST) In-Reply-To: <20090303045105.GA6920-at-www2.mrbrklyn.com> References: <20090303045105.GA6920-at-www2.mrbrklyn.com> Date: Tue, 3 Mar 2009 14:10:29 -0500 Message-ID: <7405d1440903031110o43f73255w50f052b5acc56e6b-at-mail.gmail.com> Subject: Re: [NYLXS - HANGOUT] Free Software Cooperative - it is in the genes From: Ronny Abraham To: hangout-at-mrbrklyn.com Content-Type: multipart/alternative; boundary=0016e643594844f0bf04643bb078 Sender: lest-hangout-at-mrbrklyn.com Precedence: bulk Reply-To: hangout-at-mrbrklyn.com
--0016e643594844f0bf04643bb078 Content-Type: text/plain; charset=windows-1252 Content-Transfer-Encoding: quoted-printable
WTF?
On Mon, Mar 2, 2009 at 11:51 PM, Ruben Safir wrote:
> March 3, 2009 > Basics > In a Helpless Baby, the Roots of Our Social Glue > By NATALIE ANGIER > > In seeking bipartisan support for his economic policies, President Obama > has tried every tip on the standard hospitality crib sheet: beer and > football, milk and cookies, Earth, Wind and Fire. > > Maybe the president needs to borrow a new crib sheet =97 the kind with a > genuine baby wrapped inside. > > A baby may look helpless. It can=92t walk, talk, think symbolically or > overhaul the nation=92s banking system. Yet as social emulsifiers go, > nothing can beat a happily babbling baby. A baby is born knowing how to > work the crowd. A toothless smile here, a musical squeal there, and even > hard-nosed cynics grow soft in the head and weak in the knees. > > In the view of the primatologist Sarah Blaffer Hrdy, the extraordinary > social skills of an infant are at the heart of what makes us human. > Through its ability to solicit and secure the attentive care not just of > its mother but of many others in its sensory purview, a baby promotes > many of the behaviors and emotions that we prize in ourselves and that > often distinguish us from other animals, including a willingness to > share, to cooperate with strangers, to relax one=92s guard, uncurl one=92= s > lip and widen one=92s pronoun circle beyond the stifling confines of me, > myself and mine. > > As Dr. Hrdy argues in her latest book, =93Mothers and Others: The > Evolutionary Origins of Mutual Understanding,=94 which will be published > by Harvard University Press in April, human babies are so outrageously > dependent on their elders for such a long time that humanity would never > have made it without a break from the great ape model of child-rearing. > Chimpanzee and gorilla mothers are capable of rearing their offspring > pretty much through their own powers, but human mothers are not. > > Human beings evolved as cooperative breeders, says Dr. Hrdy, a > reproductive strategy in which mothers are assisted by as-if mothers, or > =93allomothers,=94 individuals of either sex who help care for and feed t= he > young. Most biologists would concur that humans have evolved the need > for shared child care, but Dr. Hrdy takes it a step further, arguing > that our status as cooperative breeders, rather than our exceptionally > complex brains, helps explain many aspects of our temperament. Our > relative pacifism, for example, or the expectation that we can fly from > New York to Los Angeles without fear of personal dismemberment. > Chimpanzees are pretty smart, but were you to board an airplane filled > with chimpanzees, you =93would be lucky to disembark with all 10 fingers > and toes still attached,=94 Dr. Hrdy writes. > > Our capacity to cooperate in groups, to empathize with others and to > wonder what others are thinking and feeling =97 all these traits, Dr. Hrd= y > argues, probably arose in response to the selective pressures of being > in a cooperatively breeding social group, and the need to trust and rely > on others and be deemed trustworthy and reliable in turn. Babies became > adorable and keen to make connections with every passing adult gaze. > Mothers became willing to play pass the baby. Dr. Hrdy points out that > mother chimpanzees and gorillas jealously hold on to their infants for > the first six months or more of life. Other females may express real > interest in the newborn, but the mother does not let go: you never know > when one of those females will turn infanticidal, or be unwilling or > unable to defend the young ape against an infanticidal male. > > By contrast, human mothers in virtually every culture studied allow > others to hold their babies from birth onward, to a greater or lesser > extent depending on tradition. Among the !Kung foragers of the Kalahari, > babies are held by a father, grandmother, older sibling or some other > allomother maybe 25 percent of the time. Among the Efe foragers of > Central Africa, babies spend 60 percent of their daylight hours being > toted around by somebody other than their mother. In 87 percent of > foraging societies, mothers sometimes suckle each other=92s children, > another remarkable display of social trust. > > Dr. Hrdy wrote her book in part to counter what she sees as the reigning > dogma among evolutionary scholars that humans evolved their extreme > sociality and cooperative behavior to better compete with other humans. > =93I=92m not comfortable accepting this idea that the origins of > hypersociality can be found in warfare, or that in-group amity arose in > the interest of out-group enmity,=94 she said in a telephone interview. > Sure, humans have been notably violent and militaristic for the last > 12,000 or so years, she said, when hunter-gatherers started settling > down and defending territories, and populations started getting > seriously dense. But before then? There weren=92t enough people around to > wage wars. By the latest estimates, the average population size during > the hundreds of thousands of years of human evolution that preceded the > Neolithic Age may have been around 2,000 breeding adults. =93What would > humans have been fighting over?=94 Dr. Hrdy said. =93They were too busy > trying to keep themselves and their children alive.=94 > > Dr. Hrdy also argues that our human ancestors became emotionally modern > long before the human brain had reached its current average volume of > 1,300 cubic centimeters, which is about three times the size of a > chimpanzee brain =97 in other words, that we became the nicest apes befor= e > becoming the smartest. You don=92t need a bulging brain to evolve > cooperative breeding. Many species of birds breed cooperatively, as do > lions, rats, meerkats, wolves and marmosets, among others. But to become > a cooperatively breeding ape, and to persuade a bunch of smart, > hot-tempered, suspicious, politically cunning primates to start sharing > child care and provisionings, now that took a novel evolutionary > development, the advent of this thing called trust. > > To explain the rise of cooperative breeding among our forebears, Dr. > Hrdy synthesizes an array of new research in anthropology, genetics, > infant development, comparative biology. She notes that recent research > has overturned the longstanding insistence that humans are a patrilocal > species, that is, with women moving away from their birth families to > join their husbands. Instead, it seems that young mothers in many > traditional societies have their own mothers and other female relatives > close at hand, and who better to trust with baby care than your mom or > your aunt? New studies have also shown the importance of postmenopausal > women to gathering roots and tubers, the sort of unsexy foods that are > difficult to disinter and lack the succulent status of, say, a freshly > killed oryx, but that just may help feed the kids in hard times. Other > anthropologists have made the startling discovery that children have > entertainment value, and that among traditional cultures without > television or Internet access, a bobble-headed baby is the best show in > town. > > However cooperative breeding got started, its impact on human evolution > was profound. With helpers in the nest, women could give birth to > offspring with ever longer childhoods =97 the better to build big brains > and stout immune systems =97 and, paradoxically, at ever shrinking > intervals. The average time between births for a chimpanzee mother is > about six years; for a human mother, it=92s two or three years. As a > result of our combined braininess and fecundity, humans have managed to > colonize the planet; exploit, marginalize or exterminate all competing > forms of life; build a vast military-industrial complex all under the > auspices of Bernard Madoff and with one yeti of a carbon footprint, and > will somebody please hand me that baby before it=92s too late. > > -- > http://www.mrbrklyn.com - Interesting Stuff > http://www.nylxs.com - Leadership Development in Free Software > > So many immigrant groups have swept through our town that Brooklyn, like > Atlantis, reaches mythological proportions in the mind of the world - RI > Safir 1998 > > http://fairuse.nylxs.com DRM is THEFT - We are the STAKEHOLDERS - RI > Safir 2002 > > "Yeah - I write Free Software...so SUE ME" > > "The tremendous problem we face is that we are becoming sharecroppers to > our own cultural heritage -- we need the ability to participate in our ow= n > society." > > "> I'm an engineer. I choose the best tool for the job, politics be > damned.< > You must be a stupid engineer then, because politcs and technology have > been attached at the hip since the 1st dynasty in Ancient Egypt. I guess > you missed that one." > > =A9 Copyright for the Digital Millennium >
--0016e643594844f0bf04643bb078 Content-Type: text/html; charset=windows-1252 Content-Transfer-Encoding: quoted-printable
WTF?
On Mon, Mar 2, 2009 at 11:51 PM, Rub= en Safir <ruben-at-= mrbrklyn.com> wrote: le=3D"border-left: 1px solid rgb(204, 204, 204); margin: 0pt 0pt 0pt 0.8ex;= padding-left: 1ex;"> March 3, 2009
Basics
In a Helpless Baby, the Roots of Our Social Glue
By NATALIE ANGIER
In seeking bipartisan support for his economic policies, President Obama > has tried every tip on the standard hospitality crib sheet: beer and
football, milk and cookies, Earth, Wind and Fire.
Maybe the president needs to borrow a new crib sheet =97 the kind with a > genuine baby wrapped inside.
A baby may look helpless. It can=92t walk, talk, think symbolically or
overhaul the nation=92s banking system. Yet as social emulsifiers go,
nothing can beat a happily babbling baby. A baby is born knowing how to
work the crowd. A toothless smile here, a musical squeal there, and even > hard-nosed cynics grow soft in the head and weak in the knees.
In the view of the primatologist Sarah Blaffer Hrdy, the extraordinary
social skills of an infant are at the heart of what makes us human.
Through its ability to solicit and secure the attentive care not just of > its mother but of many others in its sensory purview, a baby promotes
many of the behaviors and emotions that we prize in ourselves and that
often distinguish us from other animals, including a willingness to
share, to cooperate with strangers, to relax one=92s guard, uncurl one=92s<= br> lip and widen one=92s pronoun circle beyond the stifling confines of me, > myself and mine.
As Dr. Hrdy argues in her latest book, =93Mothers and Others: The
Evolutionary Origins of Mutual Understanding,=94 which will be published > by Harvard University Press in April, human babies are so outrageously
dependent on their elders for such a long time that humanity would never > have made it without a break from the great ape model of child-rearing.
Chimpanzee and gorilla mothers are capable of rearing their offspring
pretty much through their own powers, but human mothers are not.
Human beings evolved as cooperative breeders, says Dr. Hrdy, a
reproductive strategy in which mothers are assisted by as-if mothers, or > =93allomothers,=94 individuals of either sex who help care for and feed the=
young. Most biologists would concur that humans have evolved the need
for shared child care, but Dr. Hrdy takes it a step further, arguing
that our status as cooperative breeders, rather than our exceptionally
complex brains, helps explain many aspects of our temperament. Our
relative pacifism, for example, or the expectation that we can fly from
New York to Los Angeles without fear of personal dismemberment.
Chimpanzees are pretty smart, but were you to board an airplane filled
with chimpanzees, you =93would be lucky to disembark with all 10 fingers > and toes still attached,=94 Dr. Hrdy writes.
Our capacity to cooperate in groups, to empathize with others and to
wonder what others are thinking and feeling =97 all these traits, Dr. Hrdy<= br> argues, probably arose in response to the selective pressures of being
in a cooperatively breeding social group, and the need to trust and rely > on others and be deemed trustworthy and reliable in turn. Babies became
adorable and keen to make connections with every passing adult gaze.
Mothers became willing to play pass the baby. Dr. Hrdy points out that
mother chimpanzees and gorillas jealously hold on to their infants for
the first six months or more of life. Other females may express real
interest in the newborn, but the mother does not let go: you never know
when one of those females will turn infanticidal, or be unwilling or
unable to defend the young ape against an infanticidal male.
By contrast, human mothers in virtually every culture studied allow
others to hold their babies from birth onward, to a greater or lesser
extent depending on tradition. Among the !Kung foragers of the Kalahari, > babies are held by a father, grandmother, older sibling or some other
allomother maybe 25 percent of the time. Among the Efe foragers of
Central Africa, babies spend 60 percent of their daylight hours being
toted around by somebody other than their mother. In 87 percent of
foraging societies, mothers sometimes suckle each other=92s children,
another remarkable display of social trust.
Dr. Hrdy wrote her book in part to counter what she sees as the reigning > dogma among evolutionary scholars that humans evolved their extreme
sociality and cooperative behavior to better compete with other humans.
=93I=92m not comfortable accepting this idea that the origins of
hypersociality can be found in warfare, or that in-group amity arose in
the interest of out-group enmity,=94 she said in a telephone interview.
Sure, humans have been notably violent and militaristic for the last
12,000 or so years, she said, when hunter-gatherers started settling
down and defending territories, and populations started getting
seriously dense. But before then? There weren=92t enough people around tor> wage wars. By the latest estimates, the average population size during
the hundreds of thousands of years of human evolution that preceded the
Neolithic Age may have been around 2,000 breeding adults. =93What would
humans have been fighting over?=94 Dr. Hrdy said. =93They were too busy
trying to keep themselves and their children alive.=94
Dr. Hrdy also argues that our human ancestors became emotionally modern
long before the human brain had reached its current average volume of
1,300 cubic centimeters, which is about three times the size of a
chimpanzee brain =97 in other words, that we became the nicest apes before<= br> becoming the smartest. You don=92t need a bulging brain to evolve
cooperative breeding. Many species of birds breed cooperatively, as do
lions, rats, meerkats, wolves and marmosets, among others. But to become > a cooperatively breeding ape, and to persuade a bunch of smart,
hot-tempered, suspicious, politically cunning primates to start sharing
child care and provisionings, now that took a novel evolutionary
development, the advent of this thing called trust.
To explain the rise of cooperative breeding among our forebears, Dr.
Hrdy synthesizes an array of new research in anthropology, genetics,
infant development, comparative biology. She notes that recent research
has overturned the longstanding insistence that humans are a patrilocal
species, that is, with women moving away from their birth families to
join their husbands. Instead, it seems that young mothers in many
traditional societies have their own mothers and other female relatives
close at hand, and who better to trust with baby care than your mom or
your aunt? New studies have also shown the importance of postmenopausal
women to gathering roots and tubers, the sort of unsexy foods that are
difficult to disinter and lack the succulent status of, say, a freshly
killed oryx, but that just may help feed the kids in hard times. Other
anthropologists have made the startling discovery that children have
entertainment value, and that among traditional cultures without
television or Internet access, a bobble-headed baby is the best show in
town.
However cooperative breeding got started, its impact on human evolution
was profound. With helpers in the nest, women could give birth to
offspring with ever longer childhoods =97 the better to build big brains > and stout immune systems =97 and, paradoxically, at ever shrinking
intervals. The average time between births for a chimpanzee mother is
about six years; for a human mother, it=92s two or three years. As a
result of our combined braininess and fecundity, humans have managed to
colonize the planet; exploit, marginalize or exterminate all competing
forms of life; build a vast military-industrial complex all under the
auspices of Bernard Madoff and with one yeti of a carbon footprint, and
will somebody please hand me that baby before it=92s too late.
--
http://www.mrbrklyn.c= om - Interesting Stuff
http://www.nylxs.com= - Leadership Development in Free Software
So many immigrant groups have swept through our town that Brooklyn, like At= lantis, reaches mythological proportions in the mind of the world =A0- RI S= afir 1998
http://fairuse.nylxs= .com =A0DRM is THEFT - We are the STAKEHOLDERS - RI Safir 2002
"Yeah - I write Free Software...so SUE ME"
"The tremendous problem we face is that we are becoming sharecroppers = to our own cultural heritage -- we need the ability to participate in our o= wn society."
"> I'm an engineer. I choose the best tool for the job, politic= s be damned.<
You must be a stupid engineer then, because politcs and technology have bee= n attached at the hip since the 1st dynasty in Ancient Egypt. =A0I guess yo= u missed that one."
=A9 Copyright for the Digital Millennium
--0016e643594844f0bf04643bb078--
--0016e643594844f0bf04643bb078 Content-Type: text/plain; charset=windows-1252 Content-Transfer-Encoding: quoted-printable
WTF?
On Mon, Mar 2, 2009 at 11:51 PM, Ruben Safir wrote:
> March 3, 2009 > Basics > In a Helpless Baby, the Roots of Our Social Glue > By NATALIE ANGIER > > In seeking bipartisan support for his economic policies, President Obama > has tried every tip on the standard hospitality crib sheet: beer and > football, milk and cookies, Earth, Wind and Fire. > > Maybe the president needs to borrow a new crib sheet =97 the kind with a > genuine baby wrapped inside. > > A baby may look helpless. It can=92t walk, talk, think symbolically or > overhaul the nation=92s banking system. Yet as social emulsifiers go, > nothing can beat a happily babbling baby. A baby is born knowing how to > work the crowd. A toothless smile here, a musical squeal there, and even > hard-nosed cynics grow soft in the head and weak in the knees. > > In the view of the primatologist Sarah Blaffer Hrdy, the extraordinary > social skills of an infant are at the heart of what makes us human. > Through its ability to solicit and secure the attentive care not just of > its mother but of many others in its sensory purview, a baby promotes > many of the behaviors and emotions that we prize in ourselves and that > often distinguish us from other animals, including a willingness to > share, to cooperate with strangers, to relax one=92s guard, uncurl one=92= s > lip and widen one=92s pronoun circle beyond the stifling confines of me, > myself and mine. > > As Dr. Hrdy argues in her latest book, =93Mothers and Others: The > Evolutionary Origins of Mutual Understanding,=94 which will be published > by Harvard University Press in April, human babies are so outrageously > dependent on their elders for such a long time that humanity would never > have made it without a break from the great ape model of child-rearing. > Chimpanzee and gorilla mothers are capable of rearing their offspring > pretty much through their own powers, but human mothers are not. > > Human beings evolved as cooperative breeders, says Dr. Hrdy, a > reproductive strategy in which mothers are assisted by as-if mothers, or > =93allomothers,=94 individuals of either sex who help care for and feed t= he > young. Most biologists would concur that humans have evolved the need > for shared child care, but Dr. Hrdy takes it a step further, arguing > that our status as cooperative breeders, rather than our exceptionally > complex brains, helps explain many aspects of our temperament. Our > relative pacifism, for example, or the expectation that we can fly from > New York to Los Angeles without fear of personal dismemberment. > Chimpanzees are pretty smart, but were you to board an airplane filled > with chimpanzees, you =93would be lucky to disembark with all 10 fingers > and toes still attached,=94 Dr. Hrdy writes. > > Our capacity to cooperate in groups, to empathize with others and to > wonder what others are thinking and feeling =97 all these traits, Dr. Hrd= y > argues, probably arose in response to the selective pressures of being > in a cooperatively breeding social group, and the need to trust and rely > on others and be deemed trustworthy and reliable in turn. Babies became > adorable and keen to make connections with every passing adult gaze. > Mothers became willing to play pass the baby. Dr. Hrdy points out that > mother chimpanzees and gorillas jealously hold on to their infants for > the first six months or more of life. Other females may express real > interest in the newborn, but the mother does not let go: you never know > when one of those females will turn infanticidal, or be unwilling or > unable to defend the young ape against an infanticidal male. > > By contrast, human mothers in virtually every culture studied allow > others to hold their babies from birth onward, to a greater or lesser > extent depending on tradition. Among the !Kung foragers of the Kalahari, > babies are held by a father, grandmother, older sibling or some other > allomother maybe 25 percent of the time. Among the Efe foragers of > Central Africa, babies spend 60 percent of their daylight hours being > toted around by somebody other than their mother. In 87 percent of > foraging societies, mothers sometimes suckle each other=92s children, > another remarkable display of social trust. > > Dr. Hrdy wrote her book in part to counter what she sees as the reigning > dogma among evolutionary scholars that humans evolved their extreme > sociality and cooperative behavior to better compete with other humans. > =93I=92m not comfortable accepting this idea that the origins of > hypersociality can be found in warfare, or that in-group amity arose in > the interest of out-group enmity,=94 she said in a telephone interview. > Sure, humans have been notably violent and militaristic for the last > 12,000 or so years, she said, when hunter-gatherers started settling > down and defending territories, and populations started getting > seriously dense. But before then? There weren=92t enough people around to > wage wars. By the latest estimates, the average population size during > the hundreds of thousands of years of human evolution that preceded the > Neolithic Age may have been around 2,000 breeding adults. =93What would > humans have been fighting over?=94 Dr. Hrdy said. =93They were too busy > trying to keep themselves and their children alive.=94 > > Dr. Hrdy also argues that our human ancestors became emotionally modern > long before the human brain had reached its current average volume of > 1,300 cubic centimeters, which is about three times the size of a > chimpanzee brain =97 in other words, that we became the nicest apes befor= e > becoming the smartest. You don=92t need a bulging brain to evolve > cooperative breeding. Many species of birds breed cooperatively, as do > lions, rats, meerkats, wolves and marmosets, among others. But to become > a cooperatively breeding ape, and to persuade a bunch of smart, > hot-tempered, suspicious, politically cunning primates to start sharing > child care and provisionings, now that took a novel evolutionary > development, the advent of this thing called trust. > > To explain the rise of cooperative breeding among our forebears, Dr. > Hrdy synthesizes an array of new research in anthropology, genetics, > infant development, comparative biology. She notes that recent research > has overturned the longstanding insistence that humans are a patrilocal > species, that is, with women moving away from their birth families to > join their husbands. Instead, it seems that young mothers in many > traditional societies have their own mothers and other female relatives > close at hand, and who better to trust with baby care than your mom or > your aunt? New studies have also shown the importance of postmenopausal > women to gathering roots and tubers, the sort of unsexy foods that are > difficult to disinter and lack the succulent status of, say, a freshly > killed oryx, but that just may help feed the kids in hard times. Other > anthropologists have made the startling discovery that children have > entertainment value, and that among traditional cultures without > television or Internet access, a bobble-headed baby is the best show in > town. > > However cooperative breeding got started, its impact on human evolution > was profound. With helpers in the nest, women could give birth to > offspring with ever longer childhoods =97 the better to build big brains > and stout immune systems =97 and, paradoxically, at ever shrinking > intervals. The average time between births for a chimpanzee mother is > about six years; for a human mother, it=92s two or three years. As a > result of our combined braininess and fecundity, humans have managed to > colonize the planet; exploit, marginalize or exterminate all competing > forms of life; build a vast military-industrial complex all under the > auspices of Bernard Madoff and with one yeti of a carbon footprint, and > will somebody please hand me that baby before it=92s too late. > > -- > http://www.mrbrklyn.com - Interesting Stuff > http://www.nylxs.com - Leadership Development in Free Software > > So many immigrant groups have swept through our town that Brooklyn, like > Atlantis, reaches mythological proportions in the mind of the world - RI > Safir 1998 > > http://fairuse.nylxs.com DRM is THEFT - We are the STAKEHOLDERS - RI > Safir 2002 > > "Yeah - I write Free Software...so SUE ME" > > "The tremendous problem we face is that we are becoming sharecroppers to > our own cultural heritage -- we need the ability to participate in our ow= n > society." > > "> I'm an engineer. I choose the best tool for the job, politics be > damned.< > You must be a stupid engineer then, because politcs and technology have > been attached at the hip since the 1st dynasty in Ancient Egypt. I guess > you missed that one." > > =A9 Copyright for the Digital Millennium >
--0016e643594844f0bf04643bb078 Content-Type: text/html; charset=windows-1252 Content-Transfer-Encoding: quoted-printable
WTF?
On Mon, Mar 2, 2009 at 11:51 PM, Rub= en Safir <ruben-at-= mrbrklyn.com> wrote: le=3D"border-left: 1px solid rgb(204, 204, 204); margin: 0pt 0pt 0pt 0.8ex;= padding-left: 1ex;"> March 3, 2009
Basics
In a Helpless Baby, the Roots of Our Social Glue
By NATALIE ANGIER
In seeking bipartisan support for his economic policies, President Obama > has tried every tip on the standard hospitality crib sheet: beer and
football, milk and cookies, Earth, Wind and Fire.
Maybe the president needs to borrow a new crib sheet =97 the kind with a > genuine baby wrapped inside.
A baby may look helpless. It can=92t walk, talk, think symbolically or
overhaul the nation=92s banking system. Yet as social emulsifiers go,
nothing can beat a happily babbling baby. A baby is born knowing how to
work the crowd. A toothless smile here, a musical squeal there, and even > hard-nosed cynics grow soft in the head and weak in the knees.
In the view of the primatologist Sarah Blaffer Hrdy, the extraordinary
social skills of an infant are at the heart of what makes us human.
Through its ability to solicit and secure the attentive care not just of > its mother but of many others in its sensory purview, a baby promotes
many of the behaviors and emotions that we prize in ourselves and that
often distinguish us from other animals, including a willingness to
share, to cooperate with strangers, to relax one=92s guard, uncurl one=92s<= br> lip and widen one=92s pronoun circle beyond the stifling confines of me, > myself and mine.
As Dr. Hrdy argues in her latest book, =93Mothers and Others: The
Evolutionary Origins of Mutual Understanding,=94 which will be published > by Harvard University Press in April, human babies are so outrageously
dependent on their elders for such a long time that humanity would never > have made it without a break from the great ape model of child-rearing.
Chimpanzee and gorilla mothers are capable of rearing their offspring
pretty much through their own powers, but human mothers are not.
Human beings evolved as cooperative breeders, says Dr. Hrdy, a
reproductive strategy in which mothers are assisted by as-if mothers, or > =93allomothers,=94 individuals of either sex who help care for and feed the=
young. Most biologists would concur that humans have evolved the need
for shared child care, but Dr. Hrdy takes it a step further, arguing
that our status as cooperative breeders, rather than our exceptionally
complex brains, helps explain many aspects of our temperament. Our
relative pacifism, for example, or the expectation that we can fly from
New York to Los Angeles without fear of personal dismemberment.
Chimpanzees are pretty smart, but were you to board an airplane filled
with chimpanzees, you =93would be lucky to disembark with all 10 fingers > and toes still attached,=94 Dr. Hrdy writes.
Our capacity to cooperate in groups, to empathize with others and to
wonder what others are thinking and feeling =97 all these traits, Dr. Hrdy<= br> argues, probably arose in response to the selective pressures of being
in a cooperatively breeding social group, and the need to trust and rely > on others and be deemed trustworthy and reliable in turn. Babies became
adorable and keen to make connections with every passing adult gaze.
Mothers became willing to play pass the baby. Dr. Hrdy points out that
mother chimpanzees and gorillas jealously hold on to their infants for
the first six months or more of life. Other females may express real
interest in the newborn, but the mother does not let go: you never know
when one of those females will turn infanticidal, or be unwilling or
unable to defend the young ape against an infanticidal male.
By contrast, human mothers in virtually every culture studied allow
others to hold their babies from birth onward, to a greater or lesser
extent depending on tradition. Among the !Kung foragers of the Kalahari, > babies are held by a father, grandmother, older sibling or some other
allomother maybe 25 percent of the time. Among the Efe foragers of
Central Africa, babies spend 60 percent of their daylight hours being
toted around by somebody other than their mother. In 87 percent of
foraging societies, mothers sometimes suckle each other=92s children,
another remarkable display of social trust.
Dr. Hrdy wrote her book in part to counter what she sees as the reigning > dogma among evolutionary scholars that humans evolved their extreme
sociality and cooperative behavior to better compete with other humans.
=93I=92m not comfortable accepting this idea that the origins of
hypersociality can be found in warfare, or that in-group amity arose in
the interest of out-group enmity,=94 she said in a telephone interview.
Sure, humans have been notably violent and militaristic for the last
12,000 or so years, she said, when hunter-gatherers started settling
down and defending territories, and populations started getting
seriously dense. But before then? There weren=92t enough people around tor> wage wars. By the latest estimates, the average population size during
the hundreds of thousands of years of human evolution that preceded the
Neolithic Age may have been around 2,000 breeding adults. =93What would
humans have been fighting over?=94 Dr. Hrdy said. =93They were too busy
trying to keep themselves and their children alive.=94
Dr. Hrdy also argues that our human ancestors became emotionally modern
long before the human brain had reached its current average volume of
1,300 cubic centimeters, which is about three times the size of a
chimpanzee brain =97 in other words, that we became the nicest apes before<= br> becoming the smartest. You don=92t need a bulging brain to evolve
cooperative breeding. Many species of birds breed cooperatively, as do
lions, rats, meerkats, wolves and marmosets, among others. But to become > a cooperatively breeding ape, and to persuade a bunch of smart,
hot-tempered, suspicious, politically cunning primates to start sharing
child care and provisionings, now that took a novel evolutionary
development, the advent of this thing called trust.
To explain the rise of cooperative breeding among our forebears, Dr.
Hrdy synthesizes an array of new research in anthropology, genetics,
infant development, comparative biology. She notes that recent research
has overturned the longstanding insistence that humans are a patrilocal
species, that is, with women moving away from their birth families to
join their husbands. Instead, it seems that young mothers in many
traditional societies have their own mothers and other female relatives
close at hand, and who better to trust with baby care than your mom or
your aunt? New studies have also shown the importance of postmenopausal
women to gathering roots and tubers, the sort of unsexy foods that are
difficult to disinter and lack the succulent status of, say, a freshly
killed oryx, but that just may help feed the kids in hard times. Other
anthropologists have made the startling discovery that children have
entertainment value, and that among traditional cultures without
television or Internet access, a bobble-headed baby is the best show in
town.
However cooperative breeding got started, its impact on human evolution
was profound. With helpers in the nest, women could give birth to
offspring with ever longer childhoods =97 the better to build big brains > and stout immune systems =97 and, paradoxically, at ever shrinking
intervals. The average time between births for a chimpanzee mother is
about six years; for a human mother, it=92s two or three years. As a
result of our combined braininess and fecundity, humans have managed to
colonize the planet; exploit, marginalize or exterminate all competing
forms of life; build a vast military-industrial complex all under the
auspices of Bernard Madoff and with one yeti of a carbon footprint, and
will somebody please hand me that baby before it=92s too late.
--
http://www.mrbrklyn.c= om - Interesting Stuff
http://www.nylxs.com= - Leadership Development in Free Software
So many immigrant groups have swept through our town that Brooklyn, like At= lantis, reaches mythological proportions in the mind of the world =A0- RI S= afir 1998
http://fairuse.nylxs= .com =A0DRM is THEFT - We are the STAKEHOLDERS - RI Safir 2002
"Yeah - I write Free Software...so SUE ME"
"The tremendous problem we face is that we are becoming sharecroppers = to our own cultural heritage -- we need the ability to participate in our o= wn society."
"> I'm an engineer. I choose the best tool for the job, politic= s be damned.<
You must be a stupid engineer then, because politcs and technology have bee= n attached at the hip since the 1st dynasty in Ancient Egypt. =A0I guess yo= u missed that one."
=A9 Copyright for the Digital Millennium
--0016e643594844f0bf04643bb078--
|
|